Scientists at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences (MPI CBS) in Leipzig, Germany, observed that babies succeed in associating a meaning with a word between the age of 6 and 8 months—a capability that until now was known for older children and adults. Memory that is assigned to the meaning of words passes through the same stages during sleep that also happen in typical lexical development: So-called protowords that combine only simultaneously occurring visual and acoustic stimuli become real words that are connected to content.

The scientists investigated these relations by introducing 6- to 8-month-old infants to fantasy objects to which they gave fantasy names, such as “Bofel” or “Zuser.” Objects that differed only in form or color were called the same names, just as cats are called “cats” although they differ in their details. The researchers chose these fictitious objects to make sure that the young study participants could not access any existing knowledge.

From the infants’ brain reaction it was clear that the children could not connect new objects of the same category with the corresponding name. That means they did not recognize a new Bofel as a “Bofel” although it was quite similar to the previously seen Bofel versions. For the babies, every new object-word pair was unknown and unique, they could not yet build a general relation between them.

This changed after a midday nap. In babies who fell asleep after the learning phases, the brain could differentiate between the right and wrong term for a new object. They had consolidated their knowledge while sleeping. Babies that stayed awake could not manage to do so.

Interestingly, the children developed 2 different types of knowledge depending on the duration of sleep. After a half-hour nap they showed a brain reaction which 3-month-olds already have after associating a visual stimulus with an acoustic one. During their nap they filtered similar features out of the objects and connected them with the sound of a word. Similar to the 3-month-old babies, they perceived the word as a random sound with no meaning.

Unlike the infants who napped for half an hour, babies that slept for about 50 minutes showed a brain reaction that was previously only known for older children and adults. Here, the so-called N400 component occurred, which signals that incongruous meanings were processed in the brain—whether it be in sentences, word pairs, picture stories, or object-word pairs. By means of this component the researchers were able to recognize that the young participants in fact learned the meaning of the words.

“Our results demonstrate that children hold real word meanings in their long-term memory much earlier than assumed. Although the brain structures relevant for this type of memory are not fully matured, they can already be used to a distinguishable extent,” says Angela D. Friederici, director at MPI CBS and senior author of the underlying study which has recently been published in Current Biology, in a release.

In this context, a stage of sleep could be of particular importance: The duration of the second of the four stages of sleep, in particular, seems to have an important influence on the development of lexical memory. “During this light sleep, the transition from a simple early developing form of lexical memory to an advanced later developing form evidently takes place,” says study leader Manuela Friedrich. “These two types of memory which develop during sleep are comparable with those that we know from infant development. Whereas during sleep there are just minutes in between the two types, in typical development there are months.” The formation of memory content in sleep clearly takes place in fast motion.

“In our study, however, the babies received such a lot of information which they normally pick up within a longer time period,” Manuela Friedrich adds. “But only during sleep, when the child’s brain is disconnected from the outer world, can it filter and save essential relations. Only during the interaction between awake exploration and ordering processes while sleeping can early cognitive and linguistic capabilities develop properly.”

Image: After a midday nap in babies who fell asleep after the learning phases, the brain could differentiate between the right and wrong term for a new object, in this case a new version of a “Bofel” or a “Zuser.”  Credit: MPI CBS